In sport psychology, concentration refers to focusing on sensory or mental events coupled with mental effort. It therefore relates primarily to the selective attention dimension in which individuals are able to selectively process some sources of information while ignoring others. It has its roots in the oft-cited quote from William James’s 1890 book The Principles of Psychology (Vol. 1):
Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration, of consciousness are of its essence. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others. (pp. 403–404)
Within sport psychology, research interest in concentration is predominantly focused in two areas. First is the study of skill failure under pressure, or choking; there is strong evidence that skill failure can result from reinvestment of conscious control processes. Second, researchers have investigated the efficacy of external and internal foci of attention during learning and in skilled performance.
Measurement
Researchers have used a range of psychophysiological tools, as well as self-report measures, to infer attentional states. These include heart rate measurement, electroencephalography (EEG), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and ratings of the extent of focus on a particular information source in experimental paradigms. In addition, dual-task paradigms have been used to measure performers’ awareness of task-relevant and extraneous information, most notably in the study of choking. Self-report measures include the Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style (TAIS), which was devised by Robert Nideffer to assess a range of personality measures, a subset of which were linked to his proposed two-dimensional model of attention. This model distinguishes between direction of attention (external, internal) and breadth of attention (broad, narrow) creating four combinations. The scale contains groups of items to measure positive attributes of attention (effective integration and effective narrowing) and items to measure negative attributes (overloading and underinclusion). Other scales that are more specifically focused include the Self-Consciousness Scale, which measures individual propensity for attending to the self, and the Reinvestment Scale (and its movement-specific and decision-specific versions), which measures the tendency to exert conscious control over movements or the decision making process.
Concentration and Choking
There is good evidence that skill failure in self-paced skills such as golf putting, basketball free throws, rugby goal kicks, and static target shooting is caused by the performers concentrating on deliberately and consciously controlling their movements. Much of the research work examining this issue has assessed the performer’s attentional state in the few seconds prior to skill execution using a variety of experimental paradigms. These experimental paradigms have included cross-sectional designs comparing individuals of different levels of ability or experience, and within-participant designs in which the performer’s concentration is directly manipulated via explicit instruction or task-related instructions. Evidence from performance measures and indirect measures of attention indicates clear differences between high-skilled and less-skilled performers, and that skilled performance breaks down when individuals reinvest conscious control over largely automatic actions. For example, explicit monitoring of motor skills has been shown to have a detrimental effect on skilled performers but does not impair, and sometimes facilitates, novice performance. By contrast, experts are able to perform a concurrent secondary task with minimal impairment to primary task performance, whereas novice performance tends to get worse. Relatedly, there is within-group evidence that skilled performers’ ability to report task-relevant and extraneous information relates to how well they have been performed on preceding trials. For example, baseball batters are better able to report bat position during a performance slump than when on a streak of good performance, implying their concentration is more directed to the swing motion during slumps.
A second line of research into concentration in sport concerns the efficacy of adopting either an external focus (on an implement or movement effects) or internal focus (on body movements) during learning and skilled performance. There is now a considerable body of evidence across a variety of tasks and using different age groups that an external focus is preferable for learning and evidence that this also extends to skilled performance. Further, the effect is moderated by distance so that a more distal external focus is more beneficial than a proximal focus. By studying task outcome alongside the movement kinematics and EMG activity, improvements in movement efficiency as well as accuracy have been reported resulting in more rapid skill acquisition and better retention. The constrained action hypothesis has been proposed to explain these findings, referring to the self-organizing nature of the motor system and the potential for an external focus of attention to facilitate high frequency unconscious adjustments. Conversely, an internal focus of attention is hypothesized to constrain the self-organizing nature of the motor system as the performer attempts to actively monitor and control the system.
Conclusion
The term concentration embodies one element of the multidimensional construct of attention. In the sport psychology literature, the term has been particularly important in the study of skill failure under pressure and in the realm of skill acquisition. Concentration strategies are a key component of the mental routines performers employ before executing self-paced skills. Proposed interventions for aiding performance, particularly under pressure, support the view that the information on which a performer concentrates in the few seconds before skill execution is a key determinant of performance.
References:
- Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.
- Masters, R. S. W., Polman, R. C. J., & Hammond, N. V. (1993). Reinvestment: A dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 655–666.
- Milton, J., Solodkin, A., Hluštík, P., & Small, S. L. (2007). The mind of expert motor performance is cool and focused. NeuroImage, 35, 804–813.
- Nideffer, R. M. (1976). Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 394–404.
- Singer, R. N. (2002). Pre-performance state, routines, and automaticity: What does it take to realize expertise in self-paced events? Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 359–375.
- Wulf, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 648–660.
- Wulf, G., & Su, J. (2007). An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in beginners and experts. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 384–389.
See also: