Up until the advent of the 20th century, a significant portion of an individual’s lifespan encompassed childhood and adolescence. It’s no surprise that the majority of contemplation, philosophical inquiry, and scientific research on human development were concentrated on these formative stages. The 20th century brought forth remarkable progress in nutrition, medical advancements, and exercise physiology, leading to unprecedented increases in life expectancy. In fact, the average lifespan surged from 47 years in 1900 to 77 years by 2002.
Why delve into historical context? This historical perspective holds intrinsic value in comprehending the study of human development. Despite the shift in life expectancy, where the duration of childhood and adolescence dwindled from encompassing half of life to less than a quarter, research remains heavily centered on these phases. Most educational institutions in our country provide courses in child psychology, adolescent psychology, or child and adolescent psychology. However, courses focused on adulthood and aging are fewer in number. Remarkably, an abundance of literature delves into childhood and adolescence, spanning the initial 13 to 18 years of life, while relatively fewer resources explore the subsequent 60 years.
Why has this realm of human development been overlooked for so long? The underlying rationale is that numerous researchers and philosophers once believed that development essentially concluded by the time adolescence was reached. Furthermore, there was an assumption that any development occurring beyond childhood and adolescence was minimal, if present at all (Cartwright, 2001). It wasn’t until the later stages of the 20th century that the domain of adulthood and aging gained the momentum necessary to challenge these prevailing viewpoints.
A Change in Focus
Contemporary researchers are redirecting their attention to the significance of life beyond adolescence, reflecting a paradigm shift that acknowledges development’s continuation post-adolescence. This transformation in the landscape of developmental research can be attributed to two pivotal factors: the “baby boomer” generation and the developmental conundrum of stability versus change.
The seismic impact of the aging “baby boomer” demographic (Rosenfeld & Stark, 1987) has been a catalyst for delving into post-adolescent development. In 1940, slightly over 9 million Americans were aged 65 or older. By 2000, this figure had surged to over 35 million, with projections reaching nearly 70 million by 2040 (U.S. Census Bureau, n.d.). The burgeoning population of adults in this generation has spurred researchers to recognize the essence of aging. In essence, researchers are now embracing the notion that development is an ongoing journey throughout the entire lifespan, transcending the confines of adolescence.
The second pivotal catalyst behind this developmental shift is anchored in the three cardinal developmental issues: (a) nature versus nurture, (b) continuity versus discontinuity, and (c) stability versus change. The age-old debate of nature versus nurture has persisted alongside discussions on human development. Fundamentally, it queries whether individuals are shaped by nature (genetics) or nurture (environmental influence). The consensus largely points to an intricate interplay between the two. Nature delineates a range of potential behaviors along a spectrum, while nurture determines one’s precise location on this spectrum due to the impact of environmental influences. While this issue retains significance, it holds less sway in the realm of adulthood and aging compared to earlier developmental stages.
The next developmental issue concerns the nature of development itself—whether it unfolds in a continuous manner (gradual and cumulative) or is marked by discontinuities (stage-like, punctuated by critical periods). Advocates of the continuous perspective posit that development unfolds progressively, with each experience building upon the previous to shape a holistic individual. For instance, a child’s journey from crawling to walking emerges as a culmination of sequential achievements. Conversely, proponents of the discontinuous model contend that certain aspects of development possess critical periods or specific timeframes. Language acquisition is a prime example, necessitating mastery prior to puberty for native-like proficiency (Pinker, 1994). This contention implies the existence of critical periods that cannot be surmounted through gradual accumulation.
The final and deeply debated developmental concern revolves around the balance between stability and change. It probes whether individuals are fundamentally consistent, evolving into refined versions of their former selves (stability), or whether they undergo transformation as they mature (change; Schaie, 1973). The stability versus change enigma has compelled researchers to shift their focus beyond childhood and adolescence, extending their gaze to encompass the entirety of adulthood and aging. The answer to this conundrum requires a panoramic understanding spanning an individual’s entire lifespan, as it cannot be deciphered solely within a fragmentary quarter of life.
Theoretical Approaches
In the annals of psychology, certain luminaries have held sway, shaping the discipline’s trajectory. The realm of developmental psychology stands as no exception, where towering figures like Piaget, Freud, Vygotsky, and Erickson are emblematic figures. Yet, an intriguing disparity persists, with most theoretical frameworks primarily spotlighting childhood and adolescence. This exploration embarks on a journey to uncover these often overshadowed theoretical perspectives, illuminating their significance in understanding the enigmatic realm of adulthood and aging.
Piaget’s (1950) cognitive development theory postulates four distinct stages that individuals traverse in their cognitive evolution. However, Piaget posited that by the juncture of puberty, individuals had reached the zenith of their cognitive complexity. In parallel, Freud’s Psychosexual Theory delineates five stages, asserting that the adult personality is a tapestry woven from the loom of conflicts resolved during the initial 12 years of life. Analogous to Piaget, Freud perceived development to culminate with the onset of puberty. Likewise, Vygotsky’s (1935/1978) Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) illuminates how individuals assimilate knowledge through environmental interactions, yet its application largely orbits children and adolescents within educational contexts.
Nevertheless, not all theoretical vanguards confine their scrutiny to early developmental epochs. Erickson (1950, 1968) proffers a psychosocial development theory, contending that social interactions drive development. Amongst his three adult stages (intimacy vs. isolation, generativity vs. stagnation, and integrity vs. despair), the majority of focus gravitates towards his initial five stages spanning early development. Paralleling Erickson, Levinson (1978) orchestrates the symphony of adulthood’s seasons through his “seasons of a man’s life.” Pioneering stages and transitions from 18 to 75 years, Levinson’s theory navigates the tributaries of adulthood. Divergent from Erickson, Levinson hones in on the midlife crisis. Intriguingly, extensions of Piaget’s cognitive paradigms emerge through Schaie and Willis (2000), Perry (1970), and others. They proffer a fifth cognitive stage, cementing the concept that cognitive maturation traverses well into adulthood.
In essence, while these theorists and their theoretical architectures for adulthood and aging may not bask in the same limelight as their counterparts focused on childhood and adolescence, their contributions are far from inconsequential. As this discourse unfolds, we delve deeper into each theorist’s tapestry, weaving their insights into a rich mosaic that casts light on the labyrinthine realm of adulthood and aging. Furthermore, the crucible of lesser-known theorists and their theoretical tapestries shall be unveiled, underscoring their crucial role in unraveling the complexities of this underexplored phase of human existence.
Methodologies
In the enigmatic realm of adulthood and aging, how do we unravel the intricate tapestry of developmental trajectories? The answer is clear: through research. As in other domains, the scaffolds of traditional research methods, encompassing experimental and descriptive paradigms, underpin our understanding of adulthood and aging. However, the complexities inherent in this phase of life necessitate the utilization of research methods tailored to address the profound developmental issue of stability versus change. This exploration embarks on a journey through three key research designs: longitudinal, cross-sectional, and the synergistic sequential approach.
The longitudinal approach, akin to a seasoned navigator, follows the same individuals over an extended period. Imagine investigating the trajectory of intelligence over a 40-year span—administering tests at ages 20, 40, and 60. While illuminating transformations over time, this method brims with challenges. Temporal investment, financial outlays, attrition, and a selective cohort often cloud its insights.
To mitigate these constraints, the cross-sectional approach emerges as a feasible alternative. This method captures snapshots of diverse age groups, studying them simultaneously. Applying it to intelligence research, 20-, 40-, and 60-year-olds undergo testing on the same day. Yet, akin to a Polaroid, this method lacks depth, unable to trace an individual’s transformation. It captures slices, but not the dynamic trajectory.
In response to these dilemmas, the sequential design, a masterful fusion of cross-sectional and longitudinal strategies, emerges. Researchers employing this technique mirror the footprints of time. Initially, they capture data from diverse age groups simultaneously (cross-sectional), followed by subsequent evaluations over time (longitudinal). This technique imparts a comprehensive canvas, bearing the advantages and pitfalls of both its predecessors.
These methodological orchestrations, intertwined with experimental and descriptive traditions, kindle the torch of comprehension in the labyrinthine domain of adult development. Insights harvested through these approaches cast a spotlight on the developmental conundrum of stability versus change, nurture a deeper understanding of theoretical frameworks and theorists, and illuminate the profound impact of adulthood and aging on the multifaceted facets of physical, cognitive, and social development.
Application and Comparison
Many theories of aging have described successful aging in terms of avoidance of disease, being actively involved in society, and being able to function well both cognitively and physically (Kinsella & Phillips, 2005). Thus, research findings can be divided into three main areas within adult development: physical, cognitive, and social.
Physical Changes
Within the realm of adult development, numerous theories have sought to delineate the contours of successful aging—a composite construct often defined by the avoidance of disease, active engagement in society, and sustained cognitive and physical functioning (Kinsella & Phillips, 2005). As research findings converge, they can be dissected into three primary spheres of adult development: the physical, the cognitive, and the social dimensions.
Bodily Changes
The canvas of human life spans a finite horizon, with researchers often asserting that the upper limit of human existence hovers around 120 years. Nonetheless, the present reality finds the average life expectancy well below this theoretical threshold. In the United States, the average life expectancy currently rests at 77.9 years, with a divergence between genders. Those born in 2004 anticipate 80.4 years for women and 75.2 years for men. A century of progress has witnessed a dynamic shift in life expectancy—markedly evident in the narrowing gap between male and female life spans. The chasm, once spanning 7.6 years in 1970, has been gradually closing: 7.4 in 1980, 7.0 in 1990, 5.4 in 2000, and today, standing at 5.2 years (U.S. Census Bureau, n.d.).
Within the expansive tapestry of adulthood, the physical narrative undergoes continual revision, its contours shaped by the sculptor’s hands of time. Culminating between 19 and 26 years of age, the pinnacle of bodily performance is reached, an ephemeral zenith before the onset of change. Research reveals that the grip of experience weakens handgrip strength by 30 percent and pinch force by 26 percent in older adults compared to their youthful counterparts (Ranganathan et al., 2001). Further, a lag in reaction times surfaces among older women, contrasting with the swifter reflexes of their younger peers (Hunter et al., 2001). Nature and nurture converge as architects, orchestrating the tempo at which these changes unfold, as certain aging markers, including motor function, arterial pressure, and respiratory capacity, are underpinned by a strong genetic underpinning (Finkel et al., 2003).
Despite the symphony of subtle decline, optimism takes root within the symposium of aging. Amidst the choreography of life, the brain emerges as a resilient conductor, birthing new neurons into existence throughout the span of existence (Gould et al., 1999). These neural saplings flourish in enriched environs, a testament to the malleability that perseveres amidst the passage of time. Grayness, baldness, and facial creases, often deemed harbingers of aging, do not hold sway over life span predictions (Schnohr et al., 1998). The mythos of decline is dispelled as studies reveal that the erosion of faculties once perceived as age-related—mobility, memory—can be decelerated, even arrested (Shute, 1997). Yet, the canvas remains speckled by societal biases, as youth, driven by unfounded fears, evades the enigma of aging. These apprehensions are nourished by media portrayals, which, although distorted, perpetuate a skewed view of the golden years (Pipher, 2002).
In the evolving tapestry of human existence, the interplay between the brushstrokes of aging and the canvas of life forms a dynamic tableau. A narrative etched with physical evolution unfolds against a backdrop of societal perceptions and scientific revelations. This mosaic reveals a multidimensional depiction that transcends superficial impressions, resonating with the inherent complexities of life’s twilight chapters.
Sleep
The tapestry of slumber, woven across the canvas of life, undergoes transformation through the journey of adulthood. Like the changing hues of a chameleon, the optimal quantum of nocturnal repose and the texture of sleep’s embrace evolve over time. Research whispers that eight hours of nightly slumber is the golden ratio, a sanctuary against the somnolent abyss. Yet, deviation from this equilibrium, either in the form of truncated or extended sleep, invites a dissonant symphony of sleep woes (Grandner & Kripke, 2004). Amidst the labyrinth of adulthood, insomnia often emerges as an unwelcome guest, particularly for older souls grappling with the challenge of initiating or sustaining sleep. This nocturnal adversary casts its shadow onto the daylight hours, sowing seeds of inattention and diminishing life’s vibrancy (Ancoli-Israel & Ayalon, 2006).
As researchers delve into the archives of slumber, they uncover the nuanced interplay of sex and age. The poetic dance between sleep and its tribulations takes on varying shades in different chapters of adulthood. The tapestry reveals that the strains of insomnia’s melody are more pronounced in the scores of older women, who report more poignant notes of sleep disturbance compared to their younger counterparts (Liljenberg et al., 1989). This symphony unveils that the older notes of existence are often punctuated by sleep’s interruptions, veering off the cadence set by younger years (Vitielo et al., 2004). The stillness of the night witnesses older adults choreographing nocturnal movements, with a tempo divergent from the youthful rhythm (Gori et al., 2004). The narrative takes a poignant turn, unveiling that even a partner’s nocturnal discord can cast a shadow over one’s own sleep, impacting the wellbeing of the twinned souls (Strabridge, Shema, & Roberts, 2004).
In the quest to quell insomnia’s restive whispers, options emerge from the pharmacopeia of remedies. Benzodiazepines, while a balm for the sleep-deprived, bring forth their own discordant notes of adverse effects (Montgomery, 2002). Amid this sea of remedies, ramelteon emerges as a harbinger of hope. A melodic resonance of melatonin, this selective agonist of receptors offers a new refrain, bearing no significant subjective effects or potential for abuse, as the verses of placebo reveal (Johnson, Suess, & Griffiths, 2006). Beyond the pharmacological medley, the stage also welcomes cognitive behavioral interventions, the luminosity of bright lights to recalibrate sleep’s timing, and the vigor of physiological interventions such as exercise (Montgomery, 2002).
In the realm of night, where dreams unfold and the mind seeks respite, the chapters of adulthood breathe life into sleep’s chronicle. As researchers journey through the labyrinth of slumber, their torch illuminates the passages of change, uncovering the essence of rest in its myriad shades. Each age etches its verses upon the parchment of sleep, weaving the patterns of dreams and wakefulness into the grand tapestry of existence.
Eating Behaviors
As the hands of time sweep across the canvas of existence, the culinary landscapes of adulthood undergo shifts that mirror the cadence of life itself. Research offers a mosaic of insights into the symphony of eating behaviors, revealing how age intertwines with appetite. Among the most vibrant dancers in this gastronomic ballet is age-related weight gain, a phenomenon observed even among the most ardent disciples of activity (Williams & Wood, 2006). The script of research sketches an undeniable truth: the passage of years inevitably traces the contours of weight gain. Yet, hope glimmers on the horizon, painted in hues of healthful habits, restrained diets, and exercise—a trio that endeavors to reduce the sway of age upon the waistline. However, the spectrum of responses to this waltz with weight extends to both ends, where the shadows of eating disorders like anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa cast an uneasy silhouette. Simultaneously, the land of abundance beckons, as the canvas of obesity in the United States is splashed with escalating numbers. In this chronicle of consumption, the prevalence of obesity scales heights with each generation: 28.5 percent for ages 20 to 39, 36.8 percent for ages 40 to 59, and 31.0 percent for ages 60 and older (Ogden et al., 2006). As the spotlight shifts to the elderly, predictions murmur of obesity’s rise from 14.6 million in 2000 to 20.9 million in 2010 (Arterburn, Crane, & Sullivan, 2004). Among the threads woven into this narrative are genetics, environment, and the shadows cast by sleep’s deprivation (Hasler et al., 2004; Friedman & Brownell, 1998).
However, amidst the shadows, a glimmer of resilience emerges in the form of research, illuminating the tapestry of positive eating habits in adulthood. The bond between satiety and satisfaction is examined through the prism of happiness (Doyle & Youn, 2000). This tale unfolds in the choices made by mature adults, revealing a penchant for nutrient-rich fare, while the sweet embrace of dessert foods finds favor with a mere 5 percent (Pierce, Hodges, Merz, & Olivey, 1987). As the sands of time trickle, dietary shifts are etched into the fabric of existence, guided by a chorus of influences: parents, partners, children, nutritional enlightenment, the ebb and flow of employment, and the tyranny of time (Lake et al., 2004). Voices sing in harmony as adults profess allegiance to healthful eating, a hymn of wellness and aesthetics (Hayes & Ross, 1987).
The culinary symphony of adulthood traverses realms of indulgence, discipline, and discovery. With each bite and sip, the pages of the gastronomic novel turn, revealing the interplay of age, environment, and the ever-evolving dance of the senses. As research guides us through this journey, the banquet of insights invites us to savor not only the flavors on the plate but also the wisdom that nourishes our understanding of eating behaviors in the tapestry of life.
Exercise
Amid the symphony of existence, the rhythm of regular exercise emerges as a potent note that orchestrates a harmonious union of physical health and longevity. The wisdom woven into the tapestry of research resonates with a powerful refrain: engaging in exercise is akin to inscribing an indelible mark against heart disease and embracing a life more fully lived (Lee, Hsieh, & Paffenbarger, 1995). Yet, this sonnet of wellness extends beyond the corporeal realm, extending its tendrils into the psyche, where it paints strokes of improved self-concept, dispels clouds of anxiety and depression, and illuminates the contours of mental well-being (DiLorenzo et al., 1999; Moses, Steptoe, Mathews, & Edwards, 1989).
The narrative of exercise, a saga that traverses the entirety of adulthood, unfurls its chapters with a particular spotlight on the golden years. Researchers delve into the intricate dance between physical activity and the quality of life for older adults, uncovering a cascade of positive outcomes that caress the realms of the psyche (Alencar et al., 2006; Gitlin et al., 2006; King, Taylor, & Haskell, 1993; McAuley et al., 2006). As the horizon of research expands, new realms beckon exploration, and the psychological effects of exercise are tested within the virtual realm. This digital odyssey paints a canvas where relaxation and tension waltz to a new tune, as participants embrace exercise through the lens of virtual reality, reporting serenity and tranquility that surpass traditional forms of engagement (Plante, Cage, Clements, & Stover, 2006).
The story of exercise unfolds as an elixir that defies the boundaries of age, weaving a narrative of vitality that speaks to the intertwined destinies of body and mind. In the grand theater of adulthood, where each heartbeat marks a step in the passage of time, the symphony of exercise resounds as a powerful melody, guiding us towards a richer, more fulfilling existence.
Substance Abuse
Within the intricate mosaic of adulthood, the enigmatic realm of substance use casts both luminous and somber hues. Amidst this tapestry, research endeavors to illuminate the nuanced intricacies of alcohol and cigarette consumption among adults, weaving together a narrative that spans a spectrum from exuberance to concern.
Drawing from the canvas of the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH) for 2005, the tapestry of alcohol use emerges in three levels of inquiry: current use, binge use, and heavy use. The participants’ voices harmonize to reveal that 51.8 percent engage in current drinking, 22.7 percent partake in binge drinking, and 6.6 percent dance with heavy drinking (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2006). This orchestration extends to the realm of early adulthood, where college students, under the allure of transition, engage in heavy drinking at a substantial rate (Johnston, O’Malley, & Bachman, 1996). A tale of sex differences surfaces as predictors of heavy drinking, bearing different nuances for men and women, mirroring the multifaceted nature of this symphony (Windle, Mun, & Windle, 2005).
As the journey continues into mature adulthood, the composition shifts. The script changes, and older adults take center stage, engaging in a nuanced dialogue with alcohol. The studies that echo through the ages point to changes in patterns, and life events emerge as dance partners, influencing the rhythm of alcohol consumption. Retirement, marriage, divorce—the chapters of life weave through the narrative, each stroke altering the melody of drinking (Perreira & Sloan, 2001). The NSDUH, serving as both narrator and spectator, paints a canvas where adults aged 60 to 64 pause in contemplation, with 47.5 percent engaging in current drinking, while their counterparts aged 65 and older join in with a 40 percent resonance (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2005). The final cadence of this chapter suggests that the guidelines for alcohol consumption resonate equally for both older men and women, the arbiters of a shared symphony (R. H. Moss, Brennan, Schutte, & B. S. Moss, 2004).
As the spotlight shifts, the tapestry of substance use unveils its cigarette-clad visage, a labyrinthine journey through the currents of smoking. NSDUH weaves a tale of over 60 million current cigarette smokers, a symphony of 24.9 percent of the population, tracing a trajectory of decreased numbers yet lingering concerns (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2005). Within this narrative, echoes resonate of chronic diseases and the specter of smoking-related deaths, painting a portrait of shadows cast by this habit (Merrill & Verbrugge, 1999; Bartecchi, Mackenzie, & Schrier, 1995).
In the grand narrative of adulthood, the dance of substance use unfurls as an intricate choreography, its steps both exuberant and cautionary. It is within this dance that research strives to decipher the codes, discerning the intricate melodies that play across the spectrum of human experience.
Health and Diseases
As the pages of life turn, the narrative of health and disease takes center stage, casting its ever-shifting spotlight on the human journey. The curtain rises with a shift in the nature of afflictions. No longer plagued by the ephemeral ailments of youth like the common cold or allergies, adults traverse a new terrain marked by the slow onset and enduring presence of chronic disorders. Chronic disorders, the protagonists of this chapter, stride onto the scene with their quiet but persistent demeanor, leaving their mark as the years unfold.
In the tapestry of middle adulthood, the characters of chronic disorders reveal themselves, each with their own unique storylines. Gender becomes a key subplot, as the protagonists differ between men and women. Among women, arthritis, hypertension, and sinus problems take the stage, embodying the challenges faced in this era (Poa, 2006). In contrast, the men’s script is marked by hypertension, arthritis, hearing impairment, and the somber undercurrent of heart disease (Poa, 2006).
Amidst this symphony of challenges, a different melody emerges—the crescendo of mental illness. As adulthood unfurls, the spotlight shifts to reveal the peak onset of mental illnesses, with Alzheimer’s disease commanding attention. This intricate tango between memory and forgetfulness, which primarily graces those aged 60 or older, paints a poignant picture of profound cognitive shifts. Alzheimer’s, a form of dementia, casts its shadows over daily life, erasing the boundaries of recollection and leaving a fragmented narrative in its wake (National Institute on Aging, n.d.). Yet, within the eloquence of this storyline, a poignant conflict arises. The play of diagnosis unfolds as a labyrinthine journey, where the only definitive revelation comes posthumously, a poignant reminder of the depths of human understanding (National Institute on Aging, n.d.).
As the narrative threads of health and disease intertwine, adulthood emerges as a pivotal chapter in the human saga. The protagonists, chronic disorders and mental illnesses, dance upon the stage, each with their own rhythms and narratives. It is within this complex interplay that adults navigate the currents of their existence, embodying both resilience and vulnerability in the face of the ever-changing tides of health and disease.
Cognitive Changes
In addition to examining the physical changes in adulthood, research has examined how individuals change with regard to cognition (higher mental processes). When examining cognition during adulthood, research has primarily focused on the evaluation of Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development; expertise development; and intelligence, memory, and attention.
Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development
Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, a beacon in the realm of psychology, articulated a remarkable journey through the landscapes of human cognition. He contended that the crescendo of cognitive transformation culminated in the formal operational stage—a phase of abstract and logical thinking commencing around age 12. According to Piaget, this stage marked the pinnacle of cognitive maturation, as individuals harnessed the essence of thought and reasoning. A tapestry of cognitive development, woven through sensorimotor, preoperational, and concrete operational phases, found its apotheosis in the formal operational terrain.
However, the narrative of cognitive evolution does not solely align with the contours delineated by Piaget’s theory. Echoes of dissent resonate as researchers propose extensions to this cognitive odyssey. In the shadow of formal operational thinking, a new phase emerges—postformal thought. This next frontier, as expounded by Labouvie-Vief (2003), signifies a profound realization that thought is not monolithic, but rather reflective and adaptable across diverse situations. It acknowledges the interplay of emotion and cognition, underscoring that emotional coloration can inherently alter the fabric of thought, sometimes leading to solutions that diverge from the rigid paths of logic.
Venturing beyond Piaget’s legacy, scholars like Schaie and Willis (2000) step into the realm of adult cognition, heralding the premise that cognitive metamorphosis persists into adulthood. Here, the evolution lies not merely in the accumulation of knowledge, but in the nuanced application of acquired wisdom. Adulthood is depicted as a canvas where intellect is wielded with greater sophistication, embodying the resonance of experience and insight. Perry (1970) navigates through the cognitive tapestry, casting light on the qualitative shift in adolescent and adult thinking. He demarcates the qualitative divide between rigid binary perceptions of youth and the nuanced, relativistic hues that tinge adult cognition. In this cognitive landscape, the dialectic between the certainty of black and white and the subtleties of grey emerges as a defining motif.
As the curtain rises on the next act of cognitive exploration, theorists unveil models that transcend the confines of Piaget’s stages, stretching the cognitive canvas to accommodate the tapestry of adulthood. These cognitive landscapes encompass multifaceted stages, evoking echoes of Piaget while resounding with unique cadences. Here, adult cognitive evolution dances through the hues of the achieving period, social responsibility period, executive period, reorganization period, reintegration period, and the legacy period—an orchestration of cognitive symphonies that traverse the continuum of human development (Schaie, 1986).
In this tapestry of cognition, the story of human intellectual growth unfurls—a narrative both harmonious and diverse. From Piaget’s stages to the vistas of postformal thought, from the nuanced shifts of adult intellect to the vibrant mosaics of cognitive models, the exploration of cognition unveils a story that transcends the borders of age, inviting us to delve deeper into the kaleidoscope of the human mind.
Expertise Development
Amidst the tapestry of cognitive evolution, one vibrant thread remains steadfast—expertise development. As the sands of time sift through the hands of experience, the chiseled contours of expertise emerge as an emblematic facet of adult development. While debates swirl around the ebb and flow of mental processes in the aging mind, the realm of expertise stands as a steadfast enclave of transformation.
With the cadence of years, the gulf between novices and experts unfolds not solely in the reservoir of information amassed, but in the symphony of information distilled. A symposium of research attests that the hallmark of expertise rests not in the sheer volume of facts harbored, but in the artistry of their application (Shanteau, 1992). Here, experts dance along the precipice of discernment, harnessing the alchemy of experience to prune the superfluous cues, leaving only the essential. This ability, akin to a master painter brushing strokes of insight, is the fruit of a life adorned with decisions and seasoned by judicious choices.
Yet, the narrative does not conclude with the curation of cues; it quickens its pace into the realm of time. An expert’s judgment, a gem honed by the facets of time, gleams with swiftness. The contrast between novices and experts crystallizes not only in their choices but in the cadence of those choices. The novice’s gaze encompasses the entirety of the tableau, sifting through a maelstrom of cues, irrelevant as they may be. Contrastingly, the expert’s eye, polished by experience, gazes with precision—only the relevant cues find sanctuary in their discerning vision. This swiftness, this harmonious synchrony between experience and decision-making, is the signature of expertise—an ability that ages with grace, unfolding its petals through the journey of life.
Yet, as life’s tapestry unfurls, the canvas of expertise remains selective, imprinting its hues on specific domains. The ageless tides of development orchestrate the emergence of expertise in the mosaic of careers, relationships, faith, and myriad other arenas. The brushstrokes of expertise are not bestowed indiscriminately; they paint their masterpieces on the canvases molded by personal experiences and passions. As the mosaic of life expands, so does the spectrum of expertise—a rich tableau that thrives in the alcoves carved by time.
In this realm of expertise, the symphony of experience, insight, and time crescendos, unveiling the masterpiece of adult cognitive evolution. Amidst the ever-changing landscapes of the aging mind, expertise stands as a monument to the artistry of human growth—a testament to the enduring harmony between time, experience, and the ceaseless quest for mastery.
Intelligence, Memory, and Attention
Amidst the sprawling expanse of adulthood, the chambers of cognitive prowess continue to resonate with the harmonies of intelligence, memory, and attention. This symphony of mental faculties navigates the ebbs and flows of the aging mind, producing a complex and nuanced interplay that defines the cognitive landscape.
Intelligence, an anchor of cognition, stands resilient throughout the tapestry of adulthood. The beacon of cognitive capacity continues to shine brightly, with its luminance merely softened by the passage of time (Cohen & Swerdlik, 2005). While whispers of cognitive decline may emerge, they dance on the peripheries and are often delayed until the twilight years (Berg, 2000). These murmurs of change are more pronounced in domains demanding rapidity and motor skills, but they do not darken the entire canvas of intelligence (Finkel, Reynolds, McArdle, & Pedersen, 2005). An ode to activity’s vitality in the narrative of cognitive aging, research unveils that remaining active can temper the hues of decline (Lampinen, R. Heikkinen, Kauppinen, & E. Heikkinen, 2006; Mehta, Yaffe, & Covinsky, 2002).
Within the realm of intelligence, a dichotomy emerges—the juxtaposition of crystallized and fluid intelligence (Cattell, 1963). Crystallized intelligence, the symphony of acquired wisdom and skills, resonates harmoniously, impervious to the currents of time. In contrast, fluid intelligence, the virtuosity of problem-solving in novel terrains, experiences a subtle wane as the years accumulate (Christensen, 2001; Finkel et al., 2005; Horn & Donaldson, 1980; Li et al., 2004; Schroeder & Salthouse, 2004; Zimprich & Martin, 2002). Yet, as the baton of longitudinal studies takes the lead, these wanes lose their sharpness, often transformed into gentle undulations (Schaie, 2005). Beyond the traditional contours of intelligence lies the tapestry of emotional intelligence—a realm where adults continue to flourish. Emotional self-awareness, the symphony of managing emotions, the finesse of reading emotions, and the artistry of navigating relationships—these facets persist as emotional intelligence evolves (Goleman, 1995). In this mosaic of emotional resonance, women often take the lead, weaving a more intricate melody in tasks of emotion-related cognition (Austin, 2005).
Within the labyrinth of memory, a tale unfolds—of acquisition, retention, and retrieval (Melton, 1963). Episodic memory, the narrative thread of life’s episodes, entwines with semantic memory, the repository of general knowledge (Tulving, 1989). The aging mind becomes a custodian of recollections, where the echoes of yesteryears resonate with episodic memory, exhibiting a gradual dance of decline (Allen et al., 2005; Hertzog, Dixon, Hultsch, & MacDonald, 2003; Levine, Svoboda, Hay, Winocur, & Moscovitch, 2002; Spaniol, Madden, & Voss, 2006; Wingflied & Kahana, 2002). Yet, as the chapters of memory are inscribed, women continue to grace these pages with finesse. In the episodic recall and semantic fluency domains, women persistently outshine men, their mastery enduring over the passage of a decade (de Frias, Nilsson, & Herlitz, 2006).
Amidst the orchestration of intelligence, memory, and attention, the narrative of adulthood emerges—a symphony where age adorns cognition with subtlety, where the melody of expertise weaves its notes, and where the resplendence of emotional intelligence continues to bloom. Within this orchestral dance, the aging mind navigates its course, leaving behind the trails of a life lived, a mind evolved, and a symphony well-played.
Social Changes
The final topic is how adults change socially as they develop. Social development includes research on the following topics: family life cycle, sexuality, friendship, and lifestyle choices.
Family Life Cycle
As the hands of time tick ceaselessly forward, individuals traverse the intricate path of the family life cycle, a journey marked by shifts in roles, responsibilities, and relationships within the familial framework. This voyage, akin to a symphony of intergenerational melodies, encapsulates the evolution of families across stages, unveiling transformations that mold the fabric of our social bonds.
The conceptualization of the family life cycle, as outlined by Carter and McGoldrick (1989), illuminates this evolutionary trajectory through six distinct stages. This cyclical odyssey commences with the “launching” stage, where the fledgling individual takes flight from the nest of their original family, embarking upon the quest to establish their identity and aspirations. The second stage embraces the convergence of two launched individuals, crafting an entirely new family unit. The birth of children heralds the third stage, where the family hierarchy ascends, crowning the new couple as parents and propelling extended family members to new generational pedestals.
Yet, as the family narrative unfolds, the journey takes on hues of complexity and challenges. The fourth stage, the “couple with adolescents,” ushers in a tumultuous phase, where the interplay between parents and their burgeoning children echoes the desire for autonomy and ascent in the family hierarchy. Midlife, the fifth stage, witnesses the previous generation transcending into the role of launching their own offspring, juxtaposed against the mantle of caregiving for aging parents. This intersection of responsibilities illuminates the intricate dance of familial reciprocity.
Finally, the sixth stage, “couple in later life,” crowns the journey with wisdom and legacy. By this juncture, the protagonists have launched their children, often embracing the mantle of grandparenthood. The contours of family life have shifted, yielding a tapestry where meaning, connection, and continuity resonate—the hallmark of “family integrity” (King & Wynne, 2004).
Critics’ voices, however, reverberate through the corridors of this conceptual framework. The critique underscores the theory’s omission of a multiple family structure and its disregard for the sometimes asynchronous nature of age and stage transitions. Yet, amidst these resonances, the theory’s core essence endures, painting a vivid panorama of the family’s evolution.
Yet, beyond this narrative, the second half of life remains an enigma, a realm where the developmental dance of family intricacies lacks a robust theoretical resonance. “Family integrity,” a nascent concept, emerges as a beacon, encapsulating older adults’ pursuit of connection, meaning, and continuity within the ever-expanding tapestry of generations.
Investigations into family functioning reveal a mosaic of variables shaping the harmonics of familial bonds. The strands of family satisfaction, sexual intimacy, quality of life, strengths, flexible time management, social relationships, conflict resolution, and communication intricately weave the fabric of family interactions (Greeff, 2000).
In this symphony of life, the family life cycle orchestrates a melody of transitions and transformations. Through stages of launchings, unions, generational ascents, and late-life reflections, the journey molds and reshapes the family tapestry, etching a story rich in meaning, continuity, and human connection.
Sexuality
The intricate tapestry of human existence encompasses an undeniable facet—sexuality. This primal force weaves its threads through the fabric of life, shaping intimate connections and igniting passions that transcend age and time. Research illuminates the interplay between sexuality, relationship status, and societal dynamics, painting a portrait of desire, satisfaction, and the unsettling shadow of sexual violence.
Within the context of intimate relationships, the ebb and flow of sexual activity reveal intriguing patterns. Studies underscore the correlation between partnership status and sexual engagement, unveiling that individuals who are married or cohabiting tend to participate in more frequent sexual activity and report higher satisfaction compared to their single counterparts. This nexus between companionship and sexual fulfillment casts light on the intricate interplay between emotional intimacy and physical desire.
Yet, as the canvas of adulthood unfolds, darker hues come into focus. The surge in sexual activity that often accompanies this life stage is mirrored by an unfortunate upswing in incidents of sexual violence. A disquieting statistic looms large: nearly 200,000 reported cases of rape each year, a figure that merely scratches the surface of the unreported instances. Furthermore, unsettling research paints a somber picture, revealing that approximately 27 percent of college females have encountered unwanted sexual contact, spanning a distressing spectrum from unwanted advances to coerced acts of intercourse (Gross, Winslett, Roberts, & Gohm, 2006).
As time weaves its course, the loom of sexuality remains a constant presence. Contrary to popular misconceptions, sexual interest does not wane with age; instead, it evolves and adapts to the shifting contours of life. However, the journey is not without its challenges, as physiological changes accompany the passage of time. The onset of climacteric, a midlife transition, marks a pivotal juncture. In the late 40s to early 50s, fertility takes a gradual step back, manifesting as menopause for women—a phase marked by a significant decline in estrogen production. For men, climacteric brings subtler transformations, such as modest hormone level declines that can contribute to erectile dysfunction. In this interplay of biology and age, the narrative of sexual capability takes on a nuanced tone (Crooks & Bauer, 2002).
Late adulthood heralds a phase of grace in the symphony of sexuality, characterized by a harmonious blending of wisdom and passion. As the body’s functioning gradually wanes, the frequency of sexual encounters may ebb, yet the essence of desire persists. Both men and women may grapple with challenges in achieving orgasm, a testament to the intricate interplay between the body and the mind. Research unfolds intriguing insights, disclosing that nearly half of Americans above 60 engage in sexual relations at least monthly, with a substantial portion expressing a desire for greater frequency (Koch & Mansfield, 2001/2002). The canvas of late adulthood paints a portrait of sexual expression tempered by the canvas of life, an embodiment of the enduring spirit of desire that transcends temporal boundaries.
In the grand tapestry of existence, human sexuality weaves an intricate pattern of connection, desire, and vulnerability. As individuals navigate the evolving chapters of their lives, this facet remains an integral part of the human experience, marked by the interplay of intimacy, physiological changes, and the enduring flame of desire that burns brightly through the seasons of life.
Friendships
Friendships, like threads woven into the intricate fabric of life, hold profound significance across the spectrum of human development. As individuals journey through adulthood, friendships serve multifaceted functions, offering solace, support, and a mirror to one’s self-esteem. The role of friends evolves over time, morphing in response to the phases of adulthood, and offering a tapestry of connections that reflect the essence of human interaction.
Friendships exert a dynamic influence, altering their role as individuals traverse the diverse landscapes of adulthood. In the realm of single adulthood, the reliance on friends to satiate social needs stands out, a testament to the kinship bonds that form outside of traditional family structures. As life’s chapters unfold, the importance of friendships varies, shifting to accommodate the changing roles and responsibilities of adulthood. Whether in the stages of singlehood, marriage without children, or parenthood, the supportive embrace of friends remains a constant companion, a solace for emotional fulfillment (Carbery & Buhrmester, 1998).
Even as the journey advances into the tapestry of late adulthood, the significance of friendships endures, resonating with unique characteristics that enrich the later stages of life. Exploration into the architecture of older adult friendship networks unveils the intricate underpinnings—egalitarianism, sociability, and religiosity—serving as the foundational threads of these connections (Adams & Torr, 1998). In this chapter of existence, friendships offer a haven of shared experiences and understanding, a testament to the abiding spirit of camaraderie that transcends the passage of time.
Diving into the gendered realms of adult friendships unveils a tableau of complexities. Conventional wisdom posited that women gravitate toward self-disclosure, while men engage in shared activities. However, the canvas has shifted, revealing an evolving dynamic. Recent research paints a new picture, one where both men and women equally engage in self-disclosure and extend trust to their friends. This shift is a harbinger of change, heralding a space where emotional closeness transcends traditional boundaries, weaving a rich tapestry of connection (Radmacher & Azmitia, 2006).
Beyond the brushstrokes of gender, the canvas of adult friendships expands to include a myriad of hues. The interplay between age and relationship satisfaction adds depth to the narrative, with friendships of similar ages fostering positivity and resonance. In the realms of workplaces, friendships unfold through stages, transitioning from acquaintances to confidantes. The influence of financial status also leaves its mark, shaping the contours of friendships in distinct ways. Working-class friendships are steeped in reciprocity and interdependence, sharing material goods and services, while middle-class connections resonate through shared leisure and social networks (Walker, 1995).
The tapestry of adult friendships remains a mosaic of connections, enriched by the passage of time and the nuanced hues of experience. As individuals navigate the diverse landscapes of adulthood, friendships stand as a testament to the unending rhythm of human interaction, offering solace, resonance, and the enduring embrace of companionship.
Lifestyle Choices
The tapestry of adult life is woven with a myriad of lifestyle choices that reflect the diversity and complexity of human existence. From the threads of singlehood to the intricate patterns of cohabitation, marriage, and beyond, each lifestyle choice brings forth a distinct interplay of advantages and challenges. Research delves deep into the evolving dynamics of these choices, uncovering the multifaceted layers that shape adult lives.
An Emerging Landscape: Singlehood and Cohabitation
The landscape of adult lifestyle choices is marked by shifting trends that paint a portrait of evolving values and priorities. A noteworthy trend in the United States is the inclination to embrace singlehood for an extended period. The average age of first marriage is witnessing a noticeable delay, with men and women now tying the knot around the ages of 27 and 25, respectively (U.S. Census Bureau, n.d.). The stigma traditionally associated with being single is being reshaped by the recognition of its manifold benefits. Autonomy, privacy, and the opportunity to nurture personal resources emerge as the advantages of the single life, fostering a newfound appreciation for this vibrant phase.
Similarly, cohabitation has emerged as a burgeoning lifestyle choice, marked by the intertwining of lives and relationships without the formal bonds of marriage. This evolving dynamic captures approximately 7 percent of the adult population, offering a unique perspective on companionship. While cohabitation is on the rise, it often carries a fleeting nature, with most relationships lasting no longer than a year (Hyde & Delamater, 1999). The intricacies of cohabiting relationships are etched with financial and relational prerequisites, creating a pathway fraught with considerations before proceeding to wedlock (Gibson-Davis, Edin, & McLanahan, 2005). For those who eventually transition to marriage after cohabitation, the journey is punctuated by challenges in marital satisfaction, casting a light on the complexities of this path (Booth & Johnson, 1988). As the threads of cohabitation interweave, the challenge of concluding such relationships echoes, underlining the intricate division of shared resources and goods.
Amid Matrimony and Beyond: Marriage and Divorce
The institution of marriage casts its enduring shadow across the landscape of adult life, shaping the contours of emotional connection and shared journey. Marital unions, both celebrated and questioned, are marked by nuances that shift with the changing times. The journey through marriage is laden with peaks and valleys, with divorces most likely to occur between the 5th and 10th years of marriage (National Center for Health Statistics, 2000). Predictors of divorce include elements such as low fondness, negativity, and a dearth of shared identity, highlighting the delicate balance of marital dynamics (Buehlman, Gottman, & Katz, 1992). The aftermath of divorce unveils a trail of psychological and physical challenges, underscoring the broader impact on well-being and trust (Hetherington & Stanley-Hagan, 1995).
Embarking on a parallel journey is the exploration of gay and lesbian relationships, shattering stereotypes and amplifying the resonance of shared emotions. Contrary to misconceptions, research mirrors striking similarities between homosexual and heterosexual relationships, revealing parallel paths in satisfaction, love, joys, and conflicts (Hyde & DeLamater, 1999; Peplau, Veniegas, & Campbell, 1996). The mosaic of these relationships is marked by the pursuit of enduring bonds, transcending traditional roles and asserting individuality (Peplau & Spalding, 2000). Just as in cohabitating relationships, ending a homosexual relationship echoes with shared challenges, underscoring the universal threads that bind and challenge relationships.
Crafting the Tapestry: Communication, Gender Ideology, and Longevity
The lifespan of marriages is interwoven with myriad factors that influence their course and longevity. The threads of communication and sexual satisfaction are integral to weaving the fabric of lasting marriages. Sexual satisfaction emerges as a compensatory factor, mitigating the effects of communication challenges and enriching the intimacy between partners (Litzinger & Gordon, 2005). Within the complex landscape of gender ideology, attitudinal shifts play a pivotal role in marital happiness. Men who espouse egalitarian beliefs are found to derive greater marital satisfaction, while women navigate their perceptions through the prism of marital bonds, evaluating behaviors with a nuanced lens (Kaufman & Taniguchi, 2006; Hawkins, Carrere, & Gottman, 2002).
For those who venture through the tapestry of time together, the keys to longevity in marriage emerge as treasures. The canvas of maturing love is painted with reduced conflict potential and heightened avenues for shared pleasure. Equitable mental and physical health, coupled with the dissolution of gender differences in sources of pleasure, coalesce to forge a path that defies the passage of time (Levenson, Cartensen, & Gottman, 1993).
The canvas of adult lifestyle choices is richly hued, reflecting the complexity and diversity of human existence. As individuals thread their way through the choices of singlehood, cohabitation, marriage, and beyond, the resulting tapestry offers a vibrant narrative of personal growth, relational dynamics, and the enduring power of human connection.
Summary
In the vast landscape of developmental psychology, adulthood and aging have emerged as compelling areas of research, shedding light on the intricate processes of growth and change beyond childhood and adolescence. This article has journeyed through the multifaceted dimensions of adult development, revealing a tapestry woven with physical, cognitive, and social threads.
In the realm of physical development, exercise emerges as a pivotal factor, guarding against heart disease and bolstering mental health. Research underscores the positive impact of physical activity on self-concept, anxiety, and depression, echoing its significance throughout adulthood. Even virtual reality is being explored as a medium to enhance psychological well-being during exercise. Substance abuse casts a shadow on some adults’ lives, with research illuminating patterns of alcohol and cigarette consumption. Sex differences and life events intricately shape these behaviors, offering insights into the complex interplay between psychological factors and substance use.
Mental processes in adulthood unfold against the backdrop of cognition, with Piaget’s theory undergoing scrutiny, and expertise development assuming prominence. The mastery of expertise showcases the role of experience in decision-making and judgment. Intelligence, memory, and attention navigate the terrains of aging, revealing a nuanced landscape where fluid and crystallized abilities coexist. Emotional intelligence takes the stage, highlighting a dynamic facet of cognitive growth that transcends traditional definitions.
Navigating the journey of adulthood, the family life cycle illuminates the intricate dance of transitions, placing individuals in different roles within the family hierarchy. The evolution of friendships traverses stages and sex differences, unraveling the intricate tapestry of connection and support that spans a lifetime. Lifestyle choices emerge as intricate decisions, from embracing singlehood to navigating cohabitation, marriage, and divorce. These choices, each marked by unique advantages and challenges, paint a portrait of the diverse pathways one may embark upon.
Ultimately, this article underscores that adulthood is far from a static plateau; rather, it represents a continuum of growth and change. As the aging population expands, the canvas of research opportunities widens, offering an avenue to explore the ever-evolving landscape of adult development. Just as development doesn’t halt at a predetermined age, the promise of research remains evergreen, embracing the full spectrum of the human lifespan.
References:
- Adams, R., & Torr, R. (1998). Factors underlying the structure of older adult friendship networks. Social Networks, 20(1), 51–61.
- Alencar, K., Carvalho, L., Prado, L., Vantini, A. L., Vieira, V. C., Cardoso, A. P., et al. (2006). Older people involved in physical activity benefit from water exercise, showing longer total sleep time. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 54(4), 725–727.
- Allen, P. A., Kaut, K. P., Lord, R. G., Hall, R. J., Grabbe, J. W., & Bowie, T. (2005). An emotional mediation theory of differential age effects in episodic and semantic memories. Experimental Aging Research, 31(4), 355–391.
- Ancoli-Israel, S., & Ayalon, L. (2006). Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders in older adults. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(2), 95–103.
- Antonucci, T. C. (1990). Social supports and relationships. In R. H. Binstock & L. K. George (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (pp. 205–226). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Arterburn, D. E., Crane, P. K., & Sullivan, S. D. (2004). The coming epidemic of obesity in elderly Americans. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 52(11), 1907–1912.
- Austin, E. J. (2005). Emotional intelligence and emotional information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(2), 403–141.
- Bagwell, C., Bender, S., Andreassi, C., Kinoshita, T., Montarello, S., & Muller, J. (2005). Friendship quality and perceived relationship changes predict psychosocial adjustment in early adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(2), 235–254.
- Bartecchi, C. E., Mackenzie, T. D., & Schrier, R. W. (1995). The global tobacco epidemic. Scientific American, 272(5), 44–51.
- Berg, C. A. (2000). Intellectual development in adulthood. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 117– 137). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Booth, A., & Johnson, D. (1988). Premarital cohabitation and marital success. Journal of Family Issues, 9, 255–272.
- Botkin, D. R., Weeks, M. O., & Morris, J. E. (2000). Changing marriage roles expectations: 1961–1996. Sex Roles, 42(9– 10), 933–942.
- Buehlman, K. T., Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1992). How a couple views their past predicts their future: Predicting divorce from an oral history interview. Journal of Family Psychology, 5(3–4), 295–318.
- Butler, R. N. (1996). Life review. In J. E. Birren (Ed.), Encyclopedia of gerontology (pp. 53–59). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Carbery, J., & Buhrmester, D. (1998). Friendship and need fulfillment during three phases of young adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15(3), 393–409.
- Carter, B., & McGoldrick, M. (1989). Overview: The changing family life cycle—a framework for family therapy. In B. Carter & M. McGoldrick (Eds.), The changing family life cycle (2nd ed., pp. 191–208). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
- Cartwright, K. (2001). Cognitive developmental theory and spiritual development. Journal of Adult Development, 8(4), 213–220.
- Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.
- Christensen, A., & Pasch, L. (1993). The sequence of marital conflict: An analysis of seven phases of marital conflict in distressed and nondistressed couples. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(1), 3–14.
- Christensen, H. (2001). What cognitive changes can be expected with normal aging? Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35(6), 768–775.
- Cohen, R. J., & Swerdlik, M. E. (2005). Psychological testing and assessment: An introduction to tests and measurement (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
- Crooks, R., & Bauer, K. (2002). Our sexuality (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
- Dawson, D. A., & Grant, B. F. (1993). Gender effects in diagnosing alcohol abuse and dependence. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(2), 298–307.
- de Frias, C., Nilsson, L.-G., & Herlitz, A. (2006). Sex differences in cognition are stable over a 10-year period in adulthood and old age. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13(3–4), 574–587.
- DiLorenzo, T. M., Bargman, E. P., Stucky-Ropp, R., Brassington, G. S., Frensch, P. A., & LaFontaine, T. (1999). Long-term effects of aerobic exercise on psychological outcomes. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 28(1), 75–85.
- Doyle, K. O., & Youn, S. (2000). Exploring the traits of happy people. Social Indicators Research, 52(2), 195–209.
- Erickson, E. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W.W. Norton.
- Erickson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: W.W. Norton.
- Finkel, D., Pedersen, N. L., Reynolds, C. A., Berg, S., de Faire, U., & Svartengren, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on decline in biobehavioral markers of aging. Behavior Genetics, 33(2), 107–123.
- Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., & Pedersen, N. L. (2005). The longitudinal relationship between processing speed and cognitive ability: Genetic and environmental influences. Behavior Geneteics, 35(5), 535–549.
- Friedman, M. A., & Brownell, K. A. (1998). Obesity. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mental health (pp. 111– 118). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Gibson-Davis, C. M., Edin, K., & McLanahan, S. (2005). High hopes but even higher expectations: The retreat from marriage among low-income couples. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67(5), 1301–1312.
- Gitlin, L. N., Lawton, M. P., Windsor-Landsberg, L. A., Kleban, M. H., Sands, L. P., & Posner, J. (2006). In search of psychological benefits: Exercise in healthy older adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 4(2), 174–192.
- Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
- Gori, S., Ficca, G., Giganti, F., Di Nasso, I., Murri, L., & Salzarulo, P. (2004). Body movements during night sleep in healthy elderly subjects and their relationships with sleep stages. Brain Research Bulletin, 63(5), 393–397.
- Gould, E., Reeves, A. J., Graziano, M. S., & Gross, C. G. (1999). Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science, 286, 548–552.
- Grandner, M. A., & Kripke, D. F. (2004). Self-reported sleep complaints with long and short sleep: A nationally representative sample. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(2), 239–241.
- Greeff, A. P. (2000). Characteristics of families that function well. Journal of Family Issues, 21(8), 948–962.
- Gross, a. m., Winslett, A., Roberts, M., & Gohm, C. L. (2006). An examination of sexual violence against college women. Violence Against Women 12(3), 288–300.
- Hartup, W., & Stevens, N. (1999). Friendships and adaptation across the life span. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(3), 76–79.
- Hasler, G., Buysse, D. J., Klaghofer, R., Gamma, A., Ajdacic, V., Eich, D., et al. (2004). The association between short sleep duration and obesity in young adults: A 13-year prospective study. Sleep: Journal of Sleep and Sleep Disorder Research, 27(4), 661–666.
- Hawkins, M. W., Carrere, S., & Gottman, J. M. (2002). Marital sentimental override: Does it influence couples’ perceptions? Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(1), 193–201.
- Hayes, D., & Ross, C. E. (1987). Concerns with appearance, health beliefs, and eating habits. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 28(2), 120–130.
- Hertzog, C., Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2003). Latent change models of adult cognition: Are changes in processing speed and working memory associated with changes in episodic memory? Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 755–769.
- Hetherington, E. M., & Stanley-Hagan, M. M. (1995). Parenting in divorced and remarried families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Children and parenting (pp. 233–254). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Holladay, S., & Kerns, K. (1999). Do age differences matter in close and casual friendships? A comparison of age discrepant and age peer friendships. Communication Reports, 12(2), 101–114.
- Horn, J. L., & Donaldson, G. (1980). Cognitive development in adulthood. In J. Kagan & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Constancy and change in development (pp. 445–529). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Hunter, S. K., Thompson, M. W., & Adams, R. D. (2001). Reaction time, strength, and physical activity in women aged 20–89 years. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 9(1), 32 –42.
- Hyde, J. S., & Delamater, J. D. (1999). Understanding human sexuality (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
- Johnson, B. K. (1996). Older adults and sexuality: A multidimensional perspective. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 22, 6–15.
- Johnson, M. W., Suess, P., & Griffiths, R. R. (2006). Ramelteon. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(10), 1149–1157.
- Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., & Bachman, J. G. (1996). National survey results on drug use from monitoring the Future Study: College students. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, Institute of Social Research.
- Kaufman, G., & Taniguchi, H. (2006). Gender and marital happiness in later life. Journal of Family Issues, 27(6), 735–757.
- King, A. C., Taylor, C. B., & Haskell, W. L. (1993). Effects of differing intensities and formats of 12 months of exercise training on psychological outcomes in older adults: Correction. Health Psychology, 12(5), 405.
- King, D., & Wynne, L. (2004). The emergence of “family integrity” in later life. Family Process, 43(1), 7–21.
- Kinsella, K., & Phillips, D. R. (2005). Global aging: The challenge of success. Population Bulletin, 60, 1.
- Koch, P. B., & Mansfield, P. K. (2001/2002). Women’s sexuality as they age: The more things change, the more they stay the same. SIECUS Report, 30(2), 5–9.
- Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: Affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 201–206.
- Lake, A. A., Rugg-Gunn, A. J., Hyland, R. M., Wood, C. E., Mathers, J. C., & Adamson, A. J. (2004). Longitudinal dietary change from adolescence to adulthood: Perceptions, attributions and evidence. Appetite, 42(3), 255–263.
- Lampinen, P., Heikkinen, R., Kauppinen, M., & Heikkinen, E. (2006). Activity as a predictor of mental well-being among older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 10(5), 454–466.
- Lee, I. M., Hsieh, C., & Paffenbarger, O. (1995). Exercise intensity and longevity in men. Journal of the American Medical Association, 273, 1179–1184.
- Levenson, R. W., Castensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Long-term marriage: Age, gender, and satisfaction. Psychology and Aging, 8(2), 303–313.
- Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677–689.
- Levinson, D. J. (1978). The seasons of a man’s life. New York: Knopf.
- Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W., & Baltes, P. B. (2004). Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychological Science, 15(3), 155–163.
- Liljenberg, B., Almqvist, M., Hetta, J., Roos, B.-E., & Agren, H. (1989). Age and prevalence of insomnia in adulthood. European Journal of Psychiatry, 3(1), 5–12.
- Litzinger, S., & Gordon, K. C. (2005). Exploring relationships among communication, sexual satisfaction, and marital satisfaction. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 31(5), 409–424.
- McAuley, E., Konopack, J. F., Motl, R. W., Morris, K. S., Doerksen, S. E., & Rosengren, K. R. (2006). Physical activity and quality of life in older adults: Influence of health status and self-efficacy. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 31(1), 99–103.
- McGinnis, S. L. (2003). Cohabitating, dating, and perceived costs of marriage: A model of marriage entry. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65(1), 105–116.
- Mehta, K. M., Yaffe, K. Y., & Covinsky, K. E. (2002). Cognitive impairment, depressive symptoms, and functional decline in older people. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50(6), 1045–1050.
- Melton, A. W. (1963). Implications of short-term memory for a general theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 1–21.
- Merrill, S. S., & Verbrugge, L. M. (1999). Health and disease in midlife. In S. L. Willis & J. D. Reid (Eds.), Life in midlife: Psychological and social development in middle age (pp. 77–103). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Montgomery, P. (2002). Treatments for sleep problems in elderly people. British Medical Journal, 325, 1049.
- Moses, J., Steptoe, A., Mathews, A., & Edwards, S. (1989). The effects of exercise training on mental well-being in a normal population: A controlled trial. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 33, 47–61.
- Moss, R. H., Brennan, P. L., Schutte, K. K., & Moss, B. S. (2004). High-risk alcohol consumption and late-life alcohol use problems. American Journal of Public Health, 94(11), 1985–1991.
- Murphy, J. G., McDevitt-Murphy, M. E., & Barnett, N. P. (2005). Drink and be merry? Gender, life satisfaction, and alcohol consumption among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19(2), 184–191.
- National Center for Health Statistics. (2000). Marital and divorce statistics. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
- National Institute on Aging. (n.d.) Alzheimer’s disease fact sheet. Retrieved February 20, 2017, from https://www.nia.nih.gov/alzheimers/publication/alzheimers-disease-fact-sheet
- Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Curtin, L. R., McDowell, M. A., Tabak, C. J., & Flegal, K. M. (2006). Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 1999–2004. Journal of American Medical Association, 295(13), 1549–1555.
- Patterson, C. J. (2000). Family relationships of lesbians and gay men. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1052–1069.
- Peplau, L. A. (1991). Lesbian and gay relationships. In J. C. Gonsiorek & J. D. Weinrich (Eds.), Homosexuality: Research implications for public policy (pp. 177–196). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
- Peplau, L. A., & Spalding, L. R. (2000). The close relationships of lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 111–123). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
- Peplau, L. A., Veniegas, R. C., & Campbell, S. M. (1996). Gay and lesbian relationships. In R. C. Savin-Williams & K. M. Cohen (Eds.), The lives of lesbians, gays, and bisexuals (pp. 250–273). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
- Perreira, K. M., & Sloan, F. A. (2001). Life events and alcohol consumption among mature adults: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62(4), 501–508.
- Perry, W. G. (1970). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
- Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. New York: Harcourt Brace.
- Pierce, R. P., Hodges, P. A., Merz, B., & Olivey, A. (1987). Food preferences and perceptions of institutionalized elderly: A case report. Journal of Nutrition for the Elderly, 7(1), 35–42.
- Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: HarperCollins.
- Pipher, M. (2002). The middle of everywhere. Orlando, FL: Harcourt.
- Plante, T. G., Cage, C., Clements, S., & Stover, A. (2006). Psychological benefits of exercise paired with virtual reality: Outdoor exercise energizes whereas indoor virtual reality exercise relaxes. International Journal of Stress Management, 13(1), 108–117.
- Poa, E. (2006). Trapped in transition: The complex young adult patient. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 70(1), 29–52.
- Radmacher, K., & Azmitia, M. (2006). Are there gendered pathways to intimacy in early adolescents’ and emerging adults’ friendships? Journal of Adolescent Research, 21(4), 415–448.
- Ranganathan, V. K., Siemionow, V., Sahgal, V., & Yue, G. H. (2001). Effects of aging on hand function. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 49(11), 1478–1484.
- Rosenfeld, A., & Stark, E. (1987). The prime of our lives. Psychology Today, 21(5), 62–72.
- Roy, R., Benenson, J., & Lilly, F. (2000). Beyond intimacy: Conceptualizing sex differences in same-sex relationships. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 134(1), 93–101.
- Schaie, K. (1973). Developmental processes and aging. In C. Eisdorfer & E. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult development and aging. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
- Schaie, K. W. (1986). Beyond calendar definitions of age, period, and cohort: The general development model revisited. Developmental Review, 6, 252–277.
- Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. London: Oxford University Press.
- Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (2000). A stage-theory model of adult cognitive development revisited. In R. L. Rubenstein, M. Moss, & M. H. Kleban (Eds.), The many dimensions of aging (pp. 175–193). New York: Springer-Verlag.
- Schnohr, P., Nyboe, J., Lange, P., & Jensen, G. (1998). Longevity and gray hair, baldness, facial wrinkles, and arcus sensilis in 13,000 men and women: The Copenhagen city heart study. Journals of Gerontology: Series A. Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 53A(5), 347–350.
- Schroeder, D. H., & Salthouse, T. A. (2004). Age related effects on cognition between 20 and 50 years of age. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(2), 393–404.
- Schultz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among super athletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis, and golf. Journal of Gerontology, 43, 113–120.
- Shanteau, J. (1992). How much information does an expert use? Is it relevant? Acta Psychologica, 81, 75–86.
- Shute, R. H. (1997). Multidisciplinary teams and child health care: Practical and theoretical issues. Australian Psychologist, 32(2), 106–113.
- Sias, P., & Cahill, D. (1998). From coworkers to friends: The development of peer friendships in the workplace. Western Journal of Communication, 62(3), 273–299.
- Spaniol, J., Madden, D. J., & Voss, A. (2006). A diffusion model analysis of adult age differences in episodic and semantic long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(1), 101–117.
- Strabridge, W. J., Shema, S. J., & Roberts, R. E. (2004). Impact of spouses’ sleep problems on partners. Sleep: Journal of Sleep and Sleep Disorders Research, 27(3), 527–531.
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2006). Results from the 2005 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National findings (Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H-30, DHHS Publication No. SMA 06- 4194). Rockville, MD: Author.
- Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand: Men and women in conversation. New York: Ballantine.
- Tulving, E. (1989). Remembering and knowing the past. American Scientist, 77, 361–376.
- S. Census Bureau. (n.d.). Population and Housing Unit Estimates. Retrieved February 20, 2017, from www.census.gov/programs-surveys/popest.html
- Vitielo, M. V., Larsen, L. H., & Moe, K. E. (2004). Age related sleep change: Gender and estrogen effects on the subjectiveobjective sleep quality relationships of healthy, noncomplaining older men and women. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56(5), 503–510.
- Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1935)
- Walker, K. (1995). “Always there for me”: Friendship patterns and expectations among middle- and working-class men and women. Sociological Forum, 10(2), 273–296.
- Wechsler, H., Davenport, A., Sowdall, G., Moetykens, B., & Castillo, S. (1994). Health and behavioral consequences of binge drinking in college. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 1672–1677.
- Weiss, D. J., & Shanteau, J. (2006). Do judgments alone provide sufficient information to determine the expertise of the judge who made them? Unpublished manuscript.
- Williams, P. T., & Wood, P. D. (2006). The effects of changing exercise levels on weight and age related weight gain. International Journal of Obesity, 30(3), 543–551.
- Windle, M., Mun, E. Y., & Windle, R. C. (2005). Adolescent to youth adulthood heavy drinking trajectories and their prospective predictors. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66(3), 313–322.
- Wingflied, A., & Kahana, M. J. (2002). The dynamics of memory retrieval in older adulthood. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 56(3), 187–199.
- Wiscott, R., Kopera-Frye, K., & Begovic, A. (2002). Binge drinking in later life: Comparing young-old and old-old social drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(3), 252–255.
- Zimprich, D., & Martin, M. (2002). Can longitudinal changes in processing speed explain longitudinal age changes in fluid intelligence? Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 690–695.
Back to Developmental Psychology